These skills become increasingly influential throughout a child’s education, with genetic factors playing a significant role, the new Nature Human Behaviour study has found.

The research, jointly led by Dr Margherita Malanchini at Queen Mary University of London and Dr Andrea Allegrini at University College London, suggests that fostering non-cognitive skills alongside cognitive abilities could significantly improve educational outcomes.

“Our research challenges the long-held assumption that intelligence is the primary driver of academic achievement,” Malanchini, a senior lecturer in psychology, says.

“We’ve found compelling evidence that non-cognitive skills – such as grit, perseverance, academic interest, and value attributed to learning – are not only significant predictors of success but that their influence grows stronger over time.”

The study involved a collaboration of international experts and followed more than 10,000 children from age 7 to 16 in England and Wales, employed a combination of twin studies and DNA-based analyses to examine the complex interplay between genes, environment, and academic performance.

The power of non-cognitive genetics

One of the study’s most striking findings is the increasing role of genetics in shaping non-cognitive skills and their impact on academic achievement.

By analysing DNA, researchers constructed a “polygenic score” for non-cognitive skills, essentially a genetic snapshot of a child’s predisposition towards these skills.

“We discovered that genetic effects associated with non-cognitive skills become increasingly predictive of academic achievement over the school years, in fact their effect nearly doubles between the ages of 7 and 16,” Allegrini, a research fellow, says.

“By the end of compulsory education, genetic dispositions towards non-cognitive skills were equally as important as those related to cognitive abilities in predicting academic success.”

This finding challenges the traditional view of educational achievement as determined largely by intelligence.

Instead, the study suggests that a child’s emotional and behavioural makeup, influenced by both genes and environment, plays a crucial role in their educational journey.

The role of environment

While genetics undoubtedly contributes to non-cognitive skills, the study also emphasises the importance of environment.

By comparing siblings, researchers were able to isolate the impact of shared family environment from genetic factors.

“We found that while family-wide processes play a significant role, the increasing influence of non-cognitive genetics on academic achievement remained evident even within families,” Allegrini says.

“This suggests that children may actively shape their own learning experiences based on their personality, dispositions, and abilities, creating a feedback loop that reinforces their strengths.”

Implications for education

The findings of the study could have profound implications for education.

By recognising the critical role of non-cognitive skills, schools can look to develop targeted interventions to support students’ emotional and social development alongside their academic learning.

“Our education system has traditionally focused on cognitive development,” Malanchini says.

“It’s time to rebalance that focus and give equal importance to nurturing non-cognitive skills.

“By doing so, we can create a more inclusive and effective learning environment for all students.”

The study also highlights the need for further research into the complex interplay between genes, environment and education.

By understanding these factors, educators and policymakers can develop more effective strategies to support students’ overall development and achieve better educational outcomes.

“This study is just the beginning. We hope it will inspire further research and lead to a transformation in how we approach education,” Malanchini says.


To read the study, titled ‘Genetic associations between non-cognitive skills and academic achievement over development’, click here.

The study was a collaborative effort involving researchers from multiple institutions across six countries: University College London, VU University Amsterdam, University of Bologna, Royal Holloway, University of London, University of Oslo, University of York, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Institute for Behavioral Genetics, The Max Planck Institute for Human Development, The University of Texas at Austin, Institute of Psychiatry, Psychology and Neuroscience, King’s College London, and Queen Mary University of London.

The research was funded by the Medical Research Council (MRC) of the UK Research and Innovation (UKRI).